能赢话费的拖拉杌qq升级拖拉机。 能从Q到A闯关成功,能赢2元话费

您的位置: &
2014中国人境外话费逾万亿元
优质期刊推荐工具类服务
编辑部专用服务
作者专用服务
中国机器人“闯关”
到2020年,自主品牌工业机器人年产量达到10万台,六轴及以上工业机器人年产量达到5万台以上;服务机器人年销售收入超过300亿元;培育3家以上具有国际竞争力的龙头企业,打造5个以上机器人配套产业集群……
日前,工信部、发改委、财政部联合印发《机器人产业发展规划(年)》,为中国机器人产业今后的发展描绘了蓝图,指明了方向.
年,卷(期):
在线出版日期:
本文读者也读过
相关检索词
万方数据知识服务平台--国家科技支撑计划资助项目(编号:2006BAH03B01)(C)北京万方数据股份有限公司
万方数据电子出版社您的位置: &
法律驰援:儿童拨打热线电话形成巨额话费 电信局有无责任明るさによって動きの方向が反転する色依存の錯視
 かんぽの宿?青梅(東京都青梅市)
日 10:00-10:20
明るさによって動きの方向が反転する色依存の錯視
Color-dependent anomalous motion illusion and its reversal
Akiyoshi Kitaoka, Ritsumeikan University / JST CREST
since March 8, 2013
Warning: This page includes real
animations. Please do not keep watching them for a long time. Although this page
has carefully avoided the known harmful stimuli, unknown ones might happen to be
included. Never copy them.
"Rotating red
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise when this image is presented on a bright PC display or in a print with a bright environment.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 17)
&Fudo-myo-o 2012&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise when this image is presented on a bright PC display or in a print with a bright environment.
Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2012 (December 3)
"Takonyudo"
takonyudo = octopus
Each disk appears to rotate counterclockwise when this image is presented on a bright PC display or in a print with a bright environment.
Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2009 (January 11)
Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2012 (May 20)
"Rotating red
Each disk appears to rotate counterclockwise when this image is presented in a printed image with a dark environment.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 17)
References
Yanaka, K. (2012). Enhancement of the optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion
Type V by swinging the image. Talk in the 5th Illusion Workshop, Meiji
University, Tokyo, Japan, September 18, 2012.
Kitaoka, A. (2013). Invisible color and visible color: Mystery of color
the brain feels. Talk in the Shizuoka Science Museum Ru-Ku-Ru, Shizuoka,
Japan, January 13, 2013.
A brief history of phenomenal aspects of the Fraser-Wilcox illusion group
&Kitaoka, A. (forthcoming). The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension. In Shapiro, A. and Todorovic, D. (Eds.), Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Oxford University Press.&
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Fraser and Wilcox (1979)
References
Fraser,&A. and Wilcox, K. J. (1979). Perception of illusory movement.
Nature, 281, 565-566.
Observers saw illusory motion in a stationary image which consists of repeated
luminnace gradient in a saw-tooth wave form. Some reported illusory motion
from dark to light along a luminance gradient, while others saw the reversal.
The illusion is strong in the peripheral viewing. Fraser and Wilcox (1979)
claimed that this individual difference depends on some genetic properties.
Adopted from “Alex Fraser, Geneticist and Painter”
() &access August 25, 2012& (with permission from Alan Fraser)
() was a geneticist and a painter.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Faubert and Herbert (1999)
The left disk appears to rotate clockwise while the right one counterclockwise. Faubert and Herbert (1999) reported only illusory motion from dark to
light along a luminance gradient. They attributed the illusory motion to
the assumed time difference between responses to black and white.
References
Faubert, J. and Herbert, A. M. (1999). The peripheral drift illusion: A
motion illusion in the visual periphery. Perception, 28, 617-621.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000)
The left disk appears to rotate clockwise while the right one counterclockwise.
Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000), too, reported only illusory motion from dark to light along a luminance gradient. They revealed that the illusion magnitude is a positive, nearly linear function of contrast.
References
Naor-Raz, G. and Sekuler, R. (2000). Perceptual dimorphism in visual motion
from stationary patterns. Perception, 29, 325-335.
This disk appears to rotate clockwise.
This color illusion image presented by Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000) (shown
below) might be attributable to this type.
&access August 26, 2012&
&with permission from Robert Sekuler&
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka and Ashida (2003)
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.
Kitaoka and Ashida (2003) proposed that the illusory motion is strong when
patterns are arranged in the following luminance order: from black, dark-gray,
white, light-gray, and back to black.
References
Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion. VISION (Journal of the
Vision Society of Japan), 15, 261-262.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2007)
Kitaoka (2007) classified the Fraser-Wilcox illusion into Type I, IIa,
IIb and III, in each of which the dark-to-light type was distinguished
from the light-to-dark one.
References
Kitaoka, A. (2007) Phenomenal classification of the “optimized” Fraser-Wilcox
illusion and the effect of color. Poster presentation in DemoNight, VSS2007,
GWiz, Sarasota, Florida, USA, May 14, 2007.
&Dark to light&
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.
&Light to dark&
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.
&Dark to light& and &light to dark& combined
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.
&Dark to light& and &light to dark& combined, with
color enhancement
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.
The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise
while the rest clockwise.
Type I (six disks)&1500&x&1000 (pixel)
Type I (six disks)&6000&x&4000 (pixel)
The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise
while the rest clockwise.
The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.
Type IIa (six disks)&1500&x&1007 (pixel)
Type IIa (six disks)&6000&x&4031 (pixel)
Type IIb (six disks)&1500&x&1000 (pixel)
Type IIb (six disks)&6000&x&4000 (pixel)
The upper row appears to move rightward while the lower one leftward.
Type III (two rows)&1500&x&400 (pixel)
Type III (two rows)&4500&x&1200 (pixel)Type III &4500&x&1200
The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise
while the rest clockwise.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2008a, b)
&Type IV& was added to the list.
References
Kitaoka, A. (2008a). Optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusions: a pictorial classification
by Akiyoshi Kitaoka. Talk in Workshop No.005 &Experimental studies
of anomalous motion illusions& in the 72nd Annual Convention of the
Japanese Psychological Association, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan,
September 19, 2008.
Kitaoka, A. (2008b). A new type of the optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion
in a 3D-like 2D image with highlight or shade. Journal of Three Dimensional
Images (Japan), 22(4), 31-32.
The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle rings appear to rotate counterclockwise
while the rest clockwise.
dark to light
light to dark
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (a, b)
&Type V& was added to the list.
Black & dark-gray & white & light-gray & black
Purple & light purple & light red-purple & red &
References
Kitaoka, A. (2010). Introduction to visual illusion. Tokyo: Asakura-shoten
(in Japanese).
Kitaoka, A. (2012a). Designs using the red Fraser-Wilcox illusion. Journal
of the Color Science Association of Japan, 36, Supplement, 188-189.
Kitaoka, A. (2012b) The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension. Perception
41 ECVP Abstract Supplement, page 91 (Poster September 3, 2012).
"Rotating red
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise when this image is presented on a bright PC display or on a print with a bright environment.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 17)
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2011)
「フリッカー不動明王」
左右の円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。
Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2012 (June 14)
「不動明王」
左右の円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2011 (August 29)
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Yanaka and Hirano (2011)
Yanaka and Hilano (2011) reported that the Type V illusion is enhanced
by mechanical shaking.
Each disk appears to rotate counterclockwise.
(Note that this movie is not their original)
References
Yanaka, K. and Hilano, T. (2011). Mechanical shaking system to enhance
&Optimized Fraser–Wilcox Illusion Type V&. Perception, 40, ECVP
Abstract Supplement, page 171.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Yanaka (2012)
Yanaka (2012) reported that the Type V illusion is enhanced by viewing
images under dark illumination.
(Note that this movie is not his original)
References
Yanaka, K. (2012). Enhancement of the optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion
Type V by swinging the image. Talk in the 5th Illusion Workshop, Meiji
University, Tokyo, Japan, September 18, 2012.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2013)
Kitaoka (2013) reported that the direction of illusory motion of the Type
V illusion in a printed image is reversed under dark illumination.
Each disk appears to rotate counterclockwise in a printed image. PC displays
or projector-projected images show weak effects.
References
Kitaoka, A. (2013). Invisible color and visible color: Mystery of color
the brain feels. Talk in the Shizuoka Science Museum Ru-Ku-Ru, Shizuoka,
Japan, January 13, 2013.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2013)
&Rotating red-and-purple disks&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears
to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)
&Rotating red-and-cyan disks&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears
to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 1)
&Rotating red-and-cyan disks 2&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears
to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)
&Rotating red-and-blue disks&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears
to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 29)
&Rotating red-and-green disks&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears
to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 29)
&Rotating blue-and-green disks&
Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it slightly
appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)
&Rotating gray-scale disks (movie)&
Disks appear to rotate clockwise or counterclockwise.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 15)
References
Anstis S M, 1970 “Phi movement as a subtraction process” Vision Research 10
Anstis S M, Rogers B J, 1975 “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast” Vision Research 15 957-961
Gregory R L, Heard P F, 1983 “Visual dissociations of movement, position,
and stereo depth: some phenomenal phenomena” Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 35A 217-237
Kitaoka, A. (2006). Configurational coincidence
among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception,
35, 799-806.
&Rotating gray-scale disks 2 (movie)&
Disks appear to rotate clockwise.
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 15)
Kitaoka, A. (2006) Configurational coincidence
among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception,
35, 799-806.
Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment to van Lier
and Csath& (2005)
Akiyoshi Kitaoka
Department of Psychology, Ritsumeikan University, Kyoto 603-8577, Japan
E-mail: akitaoka@lt.ritsumei.ac.jp
Macromedia Flash Player is necessary to play animations on this page.
Animation 1
Reversed phi movement (Anstis and Rogers 1975). There are four rectangles,
each of which is flanked by thin lines that are dark or light. When the
luminance of the rectangles decreases and that of the background increases
accordingly, the upper two appear to converge in motion while the lower
two appear to go apart in motion. On the other hand, when the luminance
of the rectangles increases and that of the background decreases accordingly,
the upper two appear to go apart in motion while the lower two appear to
approach each other in motion. In sum, the direction of apparent motion
is from the dark flank to the neighboring part that goes darkening or from
the light flank to the neighboring part that goes brightening. Although
the rectangles are aligned vertically, the apparent positional displacement
occurs in the opposite direction to motion. If observers cross-fuse (uncross-fuse)
the right and left rectangles, the upper rectangle appears to be in front
of (behind) the lower one when they are dark.
Animation 2
The illusory motion investigated by Gregory and Heard (1983). This appearance
is quite similar to Animation 1. The only configurational difference from
the reversed phi movement is that the luminance of the rectangles is constant.
Animation 3
The illusory motion mentioned by Gregory and Heard (1983), in which the
luminance of the rectangles dynamically changes while that of the surround
is constant. This appearance is quite similar to Animations 1 and 2.
Animation 4
Phi movement (Anstis and Rogers 1975). For panel (a), dark stationary rectangles
are placed on a bright stationary background. When the luminance of the
right flank of each rectangle increases and that of the left flank decreases,
the rectangle appears to shift leftward in motion as well as in position.
Conversely, the luminance of the right flank decreases and that of the
left flank increases, the rectangle appears to shift rightward in motion
and position. If observers cross-fuse (uncross-fuse) the right and left
rectangles, the fused rectangle appears to be in front (behind) when the
right flank of the right rectangle are dark and the left flank of the left
rectangle are dark. For panel (b), bright rectangles are placed on a dark
background. In this case, phenomena are the reversal of panel (a).
Animation 5
Consistency between the reversed phi movement and stereopsis (Anstis and
Rogers 1975). In this animation, there are two phases in motion, in which
the upper-left and the lower-right rectangles are one eighth phase in advance
of the upper-right and the lower-left rectangles. For example, when the
luminance of the rectangles increases, the upper-left rectangle appear
to go leftward followed by the upper-right one, while the lower-right rectangle
appear to go leftward followed by the lower-left one. In this period, the
cross-fused (uncross-fused) upper rectangle appears to be in front (behind)
as compared with the cross-fused (uncross-fused) lower rectangle.
Animation 6
Consistency between the apparent motion&and Caf&-Wall-like
tilt illusions (van Lier and Csath& 2005). According to dynamic
changes in luminance gradient, the flanks of the rectangle appears to tilt
dynamically. (a) The luminance gradient given to both flanks is dynamically
changed while the dark-gray rectangle and the light-gray background are
stationary. The dark part of the gradient appears to shift outward in motion
and position while the bright part appears to shift inward in motion and
position. This distortion agrees with the tilt illusion. (b) The luminance
gradient given to the rectangle is dynamically changed while the dark flanks
and the gray background are stationary. At the dark part of the gradient,
the neighboring part of flanks appears to shift inward in motion and position,
while at the bright part of the gradient, the neighboring part of flanks
appears to shift outward in motion and position. This distortion agrees
with the tilt illusion.
& & & &(b)
Animation 7
Inconsistency between the apparent motion and Caf&-Wall-like tilt
illusions. According to dynamic changes in homogenous areas, the flanks
of the rectangle appears to tilt dynamically. (a) The luminance of the
homogeneous rectangle is dynamically changed while the flanks with luminance
gradient and the gray background are stationary. The darkening rectangle
appears to attract the dark parts of the flanks inward in motion while
the brightening rectangles appears to attract the light parts of the flanks
inward in motion. This distortion disagrees with the tilt illusion. (b)
The luminance of the homogeneous flanks is dynamically changed while the
rectangle with luminance gradient and the gray background are stationary.
At the dark part of the gradient, brightening flanks appears to shift inward
in motion, while at the bright part of the gradient, darkening flanks appears
to shift inward in motion. This distortion disagrees with the tilt illusion
& & & &(b)
Animation 8
Demonstration of the apparent motion in the three elemental spatio-temporal
configurations (Figure 6). The behaviors in (a), (b) and (g) are summarized
in Figure 6a (off-center line-type configuration); those in (c), (d) and
(h) are in Figure 6b (on-center line-type configuration); and those in
(e), (f) and (i) are summarized in Figure 6c (edge-type configuration).
For further explanations, see the text.
& & & &(b)
& & & & (d)
& & & & (f)
& & & &(h)
& & & &(i)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプI」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプI?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプI」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプI?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプI」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転したり、反時計回りに戻ったりするように見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプI?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転するように見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIa」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIa」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIa」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIb」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIb」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIIb」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIII」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIII?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIII」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIII?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIII?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える(弱いが)。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転したり、反時計回りに戻ったりするように見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
「赤と緑の円盤の回転?OFWタイプIV?その2」
明るいディスプレーでは円盤は時計回りに回転したり、反時計回りに戻ったりするように見える。印刷物を暗いところで見ると反時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 18)
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2013, VSJ winter meeting)
explores this dimorphism comparing to related phenomena.
Flickering presentation of a particular order makes observers see illusory motion of two different directions.
Warning: The movies shown below include flickering patterns. Please do not keep
watching them for a long time. Never copy them.
Target & black & white & target
Target & white & black & target
Cf. Flickering presentation of a Fraser-Wilcox figure also makes observers
see illusory motion of two different directions.
A Fraser-Wilcox figure, which makes some obersvers see clockwise rotation
while makes others see counterclockwise rotation.
Warning: The movies shown below include flickering patterns. Please do not keep
watching them for a long time. Never copy them.
Target & black & white & target
Target & white & black & target
It is suggested that these dimorphisms might have something to do with
the subjective color phenomena such as the Behnham top because of the same
temporal arrangement of stimuli.
These observations might lead to clarification of motion perception, color
mechanism, as well as the role of color in motion perception in the future.
<FONT size="+3" face="Arial" color="#. Kitaoka (2013, recent study)
「水色が目立つ円盤の回転?動画」
ディスクが時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 16)
「赤と紫の円盤の回転?動画」
円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 16)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転 2?動画」
円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 16)
「赤とシアンの円盤の回転?動画」
円盤は時計回りに回転して見える。戻るようにも見える。 
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 16)
「赤とシアンの円盤の往復回転」
円盤が消える時に反時計回り、現れる時に時計回りに回転して見える。
Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 19)
黒背景と交替させると効果が弱い。特に、円盤が現れる時の回転錯視はほとんどない。
個々のフレーム
交替する背景は灰色のバージョンで、中間のフレームを1枚(50%透明?上図では中列の上から3つ目)のみとした動画。
ターゲットと灰色背景の単純な交替???円盤が消える時には回転感がある。
This study will be continued.

我要回帖

更多关于 升级拖拉机单机版下载 的文章

 

随机推荐